Any views expressed within media held on this service are those of the contributors, should not be taken as approved or endorsed by the University, and do not necessarily reflect the views of the University in respect of any particular issue.
Skip to content

Abstracts

Caroline Rowland:

What predicts how quickly children learn to talk?

There are large individual differences in the speed with which children acquire language in the early years. A popular approach is to attribute this variance to differences in the quality and quantity of the child's interactions and input; for example, to the amount of child directed speech or the number of conversational turns that adults and children use in interaction. However, even the strongest findings report only small to moderate effect sizes of linguistic input. In recent years we've been applying a constructivist approach to explaining individual variation to see how far it can take us. In this approach, language development is conceptualized as emerging from rich pre-linguistic communicative and cognitive abilities, with individual learning trajectories being shaped by interactions between environmental input, the child’s current knowledge, and the child’s learning and processing mechanisms. In this talk I illustrate some of our findings from this approach, using data from the longitudinal Language 0-5 Project, in which we followed 90 children from 6 months to 4 and a half years old, and assessed the impact of a range of socio-cognitive, cognitive, and environmental factors on individual differences in language growth.

Courtenay Norbury:

Gesture use in parents and children with (and without) developmental language disorder 

In young typically developing children, direct and indirect relationships between parent gesture, child gesture and child language acquisition have been observed. Far less is known about these relationships in atypical language development. In a series of studies, we investigated the nature of gestures in parent–child dyads for children aged 6–8 years with developmental language disorder (DLD: n = 21) relative to parents of typically developing peers (TD: n = 18) and children with low language (LL) and educational concerns (n = 21). Parents of children with DLD gestured at significantly higher rates than parents of TD children, but only during a complex interactive problem solving task. Across the entire sample, parent gesture rate was positively correlated with child gesture rate, but negatively correlated with child vocabulary. In addition, parents of children with DLD were more likely to verbally translate child gestures, and this too was negatively correlated with child language. Gesture may serve as a strategy to maximise communication success for children with language difficulties and is most evident when communication demands are high. 

Aparna Nadig:

Linguistic input to children on the autism spectrum in a multilingual society: Relationships between quantity and quality of input and child language development

Children on the autism spectrum have highly varied language profiles, with 40% exhibiting  language in the normal range or mild delays, an additional 30% exhibiting significant language delays (Pickles et al., 2014), and approximately 30% having little to no functional spoken language (Bal et al., 2016). Since many children on the spectrum face significant communication challenges, on the surface they appear not to benefit from language input in their environment in the same way as typically-developing children. Moreover, given that reduced early social attention is a defining feature of autism (APA, 2013), and that reduced attention to child-directed speech has been reported (see Filipe et al., 2018 for a review), learning from the input may not necessarily be expected.

In Part 1 of the talk I will review work from my and others’ labs on the nature of the language learning environment, or input, available to toddlers and preschoolers on the spectrum, relative to that of typically developing children. I will also present evidence indicating that children on the spectrum, as a group, do in fact make use of the language they hear, and benefit from lexically rich and syntactically complex input, as do typically developing children. Differences in input, therefore, may help explain part of the tremendous variability observed in their language development trajectories.

The aforementioned social and communication challenges lead to a commonly held belief that bilingualism could further delay language development in children on the spectrum; yet a body of research demonstrates that there is no such additional delay (see Drysdale et al. 2015 for a review). In Part 2 of the talk I review evidence showing that children on the spectrum can become proficiently bilingual when adequate language exposure is provided. Furthermore, the amount of exposure to a language has a strong, positive relationship with a child's vocabulary and morphological skills at school-age, as it does in typical development (Gonzalez Barrero and Nadig 2018).

 

Danielle Matthews:

Learning to communicate through interaction in infancy

This talk will trace the early steps of pragmatic development in human infants, from a dyadic phase, through to intentional triadic communication, early word use and later developments that support adult-like inferences. The focus will be on exploring the role of learning from experience in order to make these transitions. Evidence will be provided from the study of individual differences, from randomised controlled trials and from deaf infants growing up in families with little prior experience of deafness (and who are thus at risk of reduced access to interaction).

 

Liz Nilsen:

Children’s use of a communicative partner’s perspective when generating, repairing, and interpreting statements.

Adaptation within our social world requires the ability to effectively communicate with others. To be successful, communicators are required to simultaneously track social, linguistic, and contextual information - and manage the flow of information between themselves and their conversational partners. In particular, effective communication requires that conversational partners be attuned to each other’s mental states to adequately produce and comprehend statements, and to do so in an efficient manner. Indeed, it has been argued that no social activity requires more negotiation with the minds of others than conversation (Malle & Hodges, 2007). In this talk, children’s communicative perspective-taking, that is, the ability to use the perspective of one’s conversational partner to guide communicative behaviour, will be discussed in the context of a theoretical model outlining the developmental prerequisites that support successful communicative behaviours (Nilsen & Fecica, 2011). More specifically, findings from studies where children are in the role of speakers (i.e., producing and repairing statements), as well as listeners (i.e., interpreting others’ statements) will be highlighted. An analysis of the cognitive skills associated with these abilities will be provided, noting complexities and limitations in this field of work. Finally, implications for broader social functioning as well as for specific populations of children will be highlighted.

Leone Buckle:

To what extent does the form of speech input facilitate grammar learning?

css.php

Report this page

To report inappropriate content on this page, please use the form below. Upon receiving your report, we will be in touch as per the Take Down Policy of the service.

Please note that personal data collected through this form is used and stored for the purposes of processing this report and communication with you.

If you are unable to report a concern about content via this form please contact the Service Owner.

Please enter an email address you wish to be contacted on. Please describe the unacceptable content in sufficient detail to allow us to locate it, and why you consider it to be unacceptable.
By submitting this report, you accept that it is accurate and that fraudulent or nuisance complaints may result in action by the University.

  Cancel